Today’s New York Time’s has an op-ed article by Professor Jennifer Mnookin of UCLA, “Can a Jury Believe What It Sees?  Videotaped Confessions Can Be Misleading”  Prof. Mnookin was a member of the group that helped produced the National Academy of Sciences 2009 report, “Strengthening Forensic Science in the United States: A Path Forward,” which took a skeptical view of non-DNA forensic sciences.  In this article, Prof. Mnookin trains her skepticism on one of the chief reforms advocated by those wanting to avoid wrongful convictions: recording interrogations.  Prof. Mnookin says advocates should be careful what they wish for: recording interrogations could prove misleading.

Mnookin begins by conceding that support for electronic recording of interrogations is growing in law enforcement; “firsthand experience with recording tends to turn law enforcers into supporters.”  And recording benefits defendants as well, “because the very presence of the camera is likely to reduce the use of coercive or unfair tactics in interrogation.”  But here’s her concern:

[A]ccording to recent research, interrogation recording may in fact be too vivid and persuasive. Even seemingly neutral recordings still require interpretation. As advertisers and Hollywood directors know well, camera angles, close-ups, lenses and dozens of other techniques shape our perception of what we see without our being aware of it….When the interrogator isn’t shown on camera, jurors are significantly less likely to find an interrogation coercive, and more likely to believe in the truth and accuracy of the confession that they hear — even when the interrogator explicitly threatens the defendant.

First thing to notice: Professor Mnookin is not saying that recording interrogations is a bad idea — quite the opposite.  She says, correctly, that recording helps both law enforcement and defendants.

Second, Mnookin says that even recording interrogations under the best protocol imaginable does not guarantee the elimination of false confessions.  Remember that the (false) confessions of the Central Park Five were recorded.  Recording won’t cure all ills.

Third, the research she points to that describes the problem of “camera perspective bias” contains the solution: a requirement that the recording must include both the interrogator and the suspect in the picture.

The take away: recording interrogations represents a positive development, but we can’t just flip on the recording equipment.  We need to have proper protocols for this practice.  For example, we must require recording the whole interrogation, not just the last part in which  the suspect confesses.  (I’ve made that argument  here.)  Adding a requirement to record both interrogator and suspect makes all the sense in the world.

 

 

 

Comments
  1. Robert Rios says:

    I’ve said this many times before, There is a down side to video taping interrogations; 1) Many things that are said by the suspect is not admissible, ie; Past arrest, criminal history, etc…
    2) Suspect’s appearence; Suspect may have been involved in auto accident (hight speed chase) appearence of obvious injury, bandages, bloody clothing, etc…The jury may believe the suspect is the victim of police brutality, or if the suspect looks like a motorcycle gange member (excessive Tattoos, facial jewelry, etc… His appearence alone may suggest his guilt, on the other hand the suspect may be well dressed, well spoken, well educated, but he may be another Ted Bundy. So, people can wrongfully judge the suspect just based on what they see and preceives. My suggestion is to make two tapes, the interrogation tape and the confession tape. The interrogation tape can be used by the court for the motion to suppress as the jury has nothing to do with the decision to suppress, however, the jury can make a decision based on the evidentuary value of the confession. All to often, too many people equate admission with confession. An admission is “I did it” and a confession is “How I did it”

Leave a comment